ﺑﺎﺯﮔﺸﺖ ﺑﻪ ﺻﻔﺤﻪ ﻗﺒﻠﯽ
خرید پکیج
تعداد آیتم قابل مشاهده باقیمانده : -2 مورد

Emergent literacy including language development

Emergent literacy including language development
Author:
Teresa K Duryea, MD
Section Editors:
Marilyn Augustyn, MD
Sanghamitra M Misra, MD, MEd
Deputy Editor:
Niloufar Tehrani, MD
Literature review current through: Apr 2025. | This topic last updated: Oct 14, 2024.

INTRODUCTION — 

During their early years, children acquire knowledge and skills in informal settings that are literate behaviors, thus literacy acquisition begins long before children enter school.

This topic will review emergent literacy, including factors involved in language development.

DEFINITIONS

Emergent literacy — The American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) defines emergent literacy as the skills that children learn during early speech and language development (beginning at birth and through the preschool years) that are important to development of literacy (reading and writing) [1].

The term encompasses the following principles [2-4]:

Literacy development is an ongoing process

All aspects of literacy develop simultaneously

There is a natural hierarchy to the stages of development

Language and literacy acquisition share important features

Domains of communications — Communication has nonverbal and verbal domains. Nonverbal communication includes body language, gestures, and signing. Verbal communication occurs in oral or written modes and has three components [5]:

Receptive language – reception of sensory information

Expressive language – motoric expression; involves articulation, voice, fluency

Linguistics – CNS processing of sensory and motor functions and formulating language

Language parameters — Language has four components [6]:

Phonology – Speech sounds, phonemes

Semantics – Vocabulary, word meaning

Syntax – Grammar, combining words into sentences

Pragmatics – Use in context to communicate effectively

LITERACY DEVELOPMENT

Foundation — The acquisition of literacy requires both competent oral language skills and an understanding of the relationship between oral and written language [7]. Literacy and language are related in important ways. Both occur in a social context for the purpose of communication. Characteristics of parent/caregiver-child interactions that support language acquisition also facilitate early reading and writing development [8]. Oral language competence is highly correlated with success in learning to read [3,9].

Stages of acquisition — The development of language and the attainment of literacy skills in children occur along a continuum that has a hierarchy of stages of development, including the acquisition of oral language, reading, writing, and spelling skills [3,4,10,11]:

Most children progress toward language competence through a series of similar stages despite differences in cultural and linguistic backgrounds [3]. Children pass through these stages at different ages and in a variety of ways [2]. A child may be at a different level of competency for each domain of literacy.

The first stage begins when children learn to recognize letters as unique patterns and comprehend that print, not pictures, carries the message. The second stage begins when children become aware that letters provide clues for reading, and they may recognize some words. In the third stage, children realize sounds are determined by letters and words are created by putting sounds together. In the most advanced stages, children begin to immediately recognize morphemic parts of words [3,4,10,11].

Environment — A child's early literacy environment plays a critical role in the emergence of literacy [8,12-15]. Parent/caregiver warmth and sensitive responsiveness (ie, prompt and appropriate parent/caregiver response to the child's signals) are key elements of the early literacy environment [8,15]. Young children learn many important concepts about reading before going to school [10,16-18]. Their early experience with books and reading contributes to their later success or failure in learning to read [18,19].

Children enter school with different knowledge levels and learn to read at different rates. Those who enter school with the least knowledge of beginning reading skills are at academic risk [18,19].

Risk factors for poor reading skills among children in kindergarten include [20-22]:

Living in poverty

Living in a single-parent/caregiver household

Being from a non-English-speaking family

Lower parent/caregiver education level

Reading ability may predict change in problem behaviors during the primary school years [23].

Initiation and frequency of literacy experiences — The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends that clinicians encourage shared parent/caregiver-child reading starting at birth and continuing through early childhood [8,15]. Early initiation of and frequent exposure to shared reading is associated with increased language skills during the preschool years, school readiness, childhood literacy, interest in reading, and social-emotional benefits for both children and the parent/caregiver [8,15,24-30]. In preschool-aged children, increased frequency of shared parent/caregiver-child reading (eg, listening to stories) is directly associated with:

Teacher ratings of oral language skills at five years of age and reading comprehension at seven years of age [26]

Success on reading achievement tests in second grade [27]

Vocabulary, syntax, letter recognition, and print awareness [28]

Relational and social-emotional benefits [29,30]

Increased parent/caregiver-reported shared book reading at six months of age has also been associated with reduced parenting stress, increased parental sensitivity and warmth, and improved early parent-child relational health at 18 months of age [29]. In contrast, less frequent parent/caregiver-reported shared reading (≤4 days per week) has been associated with an increased risk of social-emotional problems in young children presenting for primary care [30].

Giving parents/caregivers developmentally appropriate books at health maintenance visits and encouraging them to read to their children increases the frequency of shared parent/caregiver-child reading [15,31,32]. Clinic-based literacy programs (eg, Reach Out and Read) are effective ways for clinicians to promote early literacy by providing developmentally appropriate books, modeling shared reading behaviors, and counseling parents/caregivers regarding the importance of shared reading and how to incorporate it into household routines [8,15]. These programs are particularly important for supporting literacy development in children who are at higher risk of poor reading and language skills due to poverty or other environmental factors [8,15]. For example, in the United States, the Reach Out and Read program provides families with bilingual books, which encourages reading in the parent's/caregiver's own language [15]. In a study including first-generation Hispanic immigrant families, receiving books at well-child visits was associated with increased frequency of shared reading; shared reading was also increased when parents/caregivers read frequently to themselves [33].

Studies have found that parents/caregivers who participate in these programs are more likely to read regularly to their children and to describe reading as a favorite parent/caregiver-child activity [8,15]. In addition, children whose parents/caregivers observed modeling and received encouragement to read aloud during health maintenance visits had higher expressive and receptive language scores. In preschool-aged children, those who were given books and encouraged to share them with their parents/caregivers in prekindergarten had greater word knowledge, spelling, and story-reading ability at the end of kindergarten compared with those who did not receive books; at the end of first grade, fewer of the children who were given books were in low reading groups [18].

Quality of literacy experiences — In addition to the frequency of early literacy experiences, the quality of the parent/caregiver-child interaction affects literacy development [12,13]. Children need access to a variety of reading materials and need to observe diverse, developmentally appropriate literate behaviors to achieve their literacy potential [8,15]. As Marilyn Jager Adams states, "it is not just reading to children that makes the difference, it is enjoying the books with them and reflecting on their form and content" [34].

Considerable interaction exists between adult reading style and the child's skill level. Various reading styles benefit different literacy skills depending upon the child's level of development. A lower-demand reading style (eg, describing and labeling) may be most appropriate for advancing vocabulary development with younger or less skilled learners, whereas a higher-demand, uninterrupted style (eg, discussion before and after the story) may be most beneficial for older or more advanced children [35].

Observational studies on the quality and style of reading report the following:

Parents/caregivers alter the interactional style of reading depending upon the child's level of communicative competence [36,37].

Children whose parents/caregivers were instructed to increase open-ended questions, expand on replies, and encourage discussions of function and attributes had higher expressive language scores and longer mean utterance than did children whose parents/caregivers did not receive this intervention [38].

The type of book that is read affects the teaching strategy of parents/caregivers; for example, greater participation from parents/caregivers and children is elicited when expository books (eg, collections of pictures or labels), rather than narrative books (eg, stories), are read [37]. In addition, although electronic books increase engagement and attention [39], they are associated with more frequent social control behaviors (eg, pushing a hand away, grabbing the book, turning away from the reading partner) and less social reciprocity (eg, conversational interaction collaboration) than print books [40,41].

Demographics — Researchers have reported variable effects of socioeconomic status, ethnicity, and parent/caregiver education on emergent literacy [12,18,42-44]:

Some researchers report that low socioeconomic and less educated parents/caregivers may be adequate teachers for their child's present level of development but less knowledgeable about language and cognitive development [45], less sensitive to the changing needs and capabilities of their children [42], and less able to foster the acquisition of prereading skills [18]. In a large cohort study, lower socioeconomic status was associated with less frequent parent/caregiver reading and fewer books at home [12].

Pediatric health care providers can help low socioeconomic status parents/caregivers foster their infants' language and cognitive development by taking advantage of parent/caregiver confidence and providing knowledge and effective strategies [45]. (See 'Suggestions for caregivers' below.)

Other researchers associate greater child-centered literacy orientation with two-parent/caregiver families, higher adult-to-child ratios, households speaking only English, ownership of more than 10 children's books, and parents/caregivers who read a few times per week themselves [43].

Still other researchers have found a wide range in the quantity and nature of literacy materials present in the homes and a large variation in the utilization of these materials, rather than a distinct difference in literacy practice by ethnicity or educational level of low-income families [44].

NORMAL LANGUAGE AND LITERACY DEVELOPMENT — 

Language and literacy skills develop in the following way:

Along several parameters simultaneously

Over an extended period of time

In a predictable order

At a predictable rate

The achievement of isolated milestones may be misleading. As an example, a child may have a large vocabulary because of a great ability to imitate or memorize yet be incapable of using these words appropriately in conversation. The normal milestones in language and literacy development are shown in the graphics (figure 1 and table 1) [46-53].

DELAYED LANGUAGE AND LITERACY DEVELOPMENT

Prevalence and etiology — Developmental language delays or disorders occur in 5 to 10 percent of preschool children. They can involve aspects of expressive, comprehensive, or total language development. The causes of delayed language development are numerous and include social deprivation, anatomic structural defects, oral motor or neuromotor dysfunction, hearing impairment, and neurocognitive delays. Delays in language and literacy development do not occur because the child is lazy, older siblings speak for them, the child lives in a bilingual environment, or the child is a twin [49]. The etiology of delayed expressive language development and speech impairment in children is discussed in more detail elsewhere. (See "Expressive language delay ("late talking") in young children" and "Speech and language impairment in children: Etiology".)

Red flags — Red flags for language or literacy problems are numerous and include [6,52,54]:

Parent/caregiver concern regarding hearing, abnormal speech production, or comprehension

No babbling by nine months

No first words by 15 months

No consistent words by 18 months

No word combinations by 24 months

Speech is difficult for parents/caregivers to understand at 24 months

Speech is difficult for strangers to understand at 36 months

Deficient lexicon (slow growth of vocabulary)

Dysfluencies (stutters) consist of more than tension-free whole-word repetitions

Child is frustrated by communication difficulty

Child is teased by peers for "talking funny"

Child avoids talking situations

Child acquires vocabulary and sentence structure but does not use language appropriately for communicative purposes

Language is unusual or confused, or ideas are not expressed clearly

Child cannot follow instructions without supplemental visual cues

Loss of milestones

Poor memory skills at five to six years of age (eg, inability to learn colors, numbers, shapes, alphabet)

Decreased linguistic awareness at five to six years of age (eg, phonemic awareness, segmentation, syntax, concepts of print)

Management and prognosis — Early identification and intervention for children with abnormal language or literacy development are critical. Children do not "outgrow" these problems and should be referred as soon as possible for evaluation and intervention. The evaluation and management of delayed expressive language development and speech impairment in children are discussed in more detail elsewhere. (See "Expressive language delay ("late talking") in young children" and "Speech and language impairment in children: Evaluation, treatment, and prognosis".)

Early identification and intervention for children whose speech and language impairment persists beyond five years of age is critical because they may continue to have difficulty into adulthood. As examples:

Children with a history of phonologic disorders perform more poorly than do controls on writing tasks; those with a language disorder in addition to the phonologic disorder have the greatest difficulty [55].

Seventy-one children with speech-language impairment at age four years were followed into adolescence [56]. Those whose speech and language impairment persisted at age five years (56 percent) were at high risk for language, literacy, and educational difficulties throughout childhood and adolescence. By comparison, when the early speech and language impairment resolved by age five years (44 percent), the outlook for spoken language development was better, but literacy skills were weak because of residual phonologic processing deficits.

Another prospective longitudinal trial compared children with speech and language impairment at age five years with age-matched controls; high rates of communication difficulty remained at 12 and 19 years of age [57]. However, only approximately 50 percent of the subjects in this study received treatment, usually during early years.

Investigations of the association of language ability and psychosocial development have mixed findings. In one large cohort study, children with poor early receptive language skills experienced more disadvantaged socioeconomic circumstances in early childhood and more behavior and psychosocial adjustment problems in the transition to adulthood than children with normal language skills [58]. In another large cohort study, late-talking children had increased internalizing and externalizing behaviors at age two years compared with control toddlers but were not at increased risk for behavioral and emotional problems at age 5, 8, 10, 14, or 17 years [59].

BILINGUAL LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT — 

Questions often arise about the language development of children reared in bilingual environments. Bilingualism is the ability to speak two languages [60]. Early research that suggested bilingual children were less intelligent than were monolingual children is incorrect. Learning two languages at once does not harm a child's cognitive abilities [61-63]; rather, it enhances them [64-67]. An initial stage of confusion, code-mixing, and comprehension-building for young bilinguals may occur but resolves [68]. At 24 to 30 months of age, bilingual children may intermix vocabulary and syntax from both languages, but vocabulary size, intelligibility, and length of utterance are normal. By 36 months of age, they become fluent bilingual speakers [49]. One should note that single-language scores in one language may not accurately reflect the child's abilities, and direct comparisons across languages and different writing systems are precarious.

Improving access to bilingual books benefits both children and their families [69]. Bilingual books enhance bilingual language development for the child, improve English as a second language for the parents/caregivers, and demonstrate respect across cultures and the value of traditions. Pediatric clinicians can play a key role in supporting parents/caregivers raising bilingual children through encouraging shared bilingual reading experiences [70].

SUGGESTIONS FOR CAREGIVERS — 

Parents/caregivers can implement the following interventions:

Speak slowly and succinctly

Label objects

Repeat, repeat, repeat

Ask open-ended questions

Expand on the child's conversation

Correct positively – Rephrase, repeat, relabel

Use interactive games and humor

Encourage the child

Tell stories to each other

Limit television viewing time

Visit the library regularly

Dialogue your activities (ie, talk about what you are doing)

Read every day [71,72]

The American Academy of Pediatrics has developed a Literacy Toolkit that includes practical tools for promoting literacy in pediatric primary care, such as tips for parents/caregivers about selecting books and sharing books with children of various ages, in addition to a policy statement for literacy promotion [8,73].

In a systematic review of 59 randomized and 17 nonrandomized clinical trials in 5848 children <6 years of age who had or were at risk for language impairment, parent/caregiver-implemented interventions (eg, naturalistic responding to child communication, asking questions and having discussions while reading with the child) were moderately associated with improved child communication, engagement, and language outcomes [74].

SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Definitions – Emergent literacy is defined as the skills that children learn during early speech and language development that are important to development of literacy (reading and writing). Literacy development is an ongoing process. Verbal communication includes receptive language, expressive language, and linguistics. Nonverbal communication includes body language, gestures, and signing. Language includes phonology, semantics, syntax, and pragmatics. (See 'Definitions' above.)

Language and literacy development – The normal development of language and literacy occurs along a continuum that has a hierarchy of stages, including the acquisition of oral language, reading, writing, and spelling skills (figure 1 and table 1). Literacy development requires competent oral language skills, which is highly correlated with reading success, and an understanding of the relationship between oral and written language. Bilingual language development does not cause delays, and clinicians should encourage shared bilingual reading experiences. (See 'Normal language and literacy development' above and 'Bilingual language development' above.)

Factors that affect the development of language and literacy include the early literacy environment, early initiation of and frequent exposure to literacy experiences (eg, shared parent/caregiver-child reading), and the quality of the parent/caregiver-child interaction. (See 'Literacy development' above.)

Delayed language and literacy development – Developmental language delays or disorders have numerous causes and can involve aspects of expressive, comprehensive, or total language development. Children should be referred as soon as possible for evaluation and intervention. (See 'Delayed language and literacy development' above and "Expressive language delay ("late talking") in young children" and "Speech and language impairment in children: Etiology" and "Speech and language impairment in children: Evaluation, treatment, and prognosis".)

Suggestions for caregivers – Parents/caregivers can promote their child's early language and literacy development by (see 'Suggestions for caregivers' above):

Speaking slowly and succinctly

Asking open-ended questions

Expanding on the child's conversation

Telling stories to each other

Limiting television time

Visiting the library

Describing what they are doing

Reading every day

  1. Roth FP, Paul DR, Peirotti AM. Emergent Literacy: Early Reading and Writing Development https://www.asha.org/public/speech/emergent-literacy/ (Accessed on July 11, 2024).
  2. Teale W, Sulzby E. Emergent literacy as a perspective for examining how young children become writers and readers. In: Emergent Literacy: Writing and Reading, Teale W, Sulzby E (Eds), Ablex Publishing Corp, Norwood, NJ 1986.
  3. Lipson MY, Wixson KK. The foundations of literacy. In: Assessment and Introduction of Reading Disability — An Interactive Approach, Lipson MY, Wixson KK (Eds), Harper Collins Publishers Inc, New York 1991.
  4. Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Child development and emergent literacy. Child Dev 1998; 69:848.
  5. Ruben RJ. Communicative disorders. The first year of life. Pediatr Clin North Am 1994; 41:1035.
  6. Flax JF, Rapin I. Evaluating children with delayed speech and language. Contemp Pediatr 1998; 15:164.
  7. Sulzby E. Writing and reading: Signs of oral and written language organization in the young child. In: Emergent Literacy: Writing and Reading, Teale W, Sulzby E (Eds), Ablex Publishing Corp, Norwood, NJ 1986.
  8. Klass P, Miller-Fitzwater A, High PC, COUNCIL ON EARLY CHILDHOOD. Literacy Promotion: An Essential Component of Primary Care Pediatric Practice: Policy Statement. Pediatrics 2024; 154.
  9. Snow CE. Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harv Educ Rev 1983; 53:165.
  10. Mason JM. When do children begin to read: An exploration of four year old children's letter and word reading competencies. Reading Research Quarterly 1980; 2:203.
  11. Frith U. A developmental framework dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia 1986; 36:69.
  12. Larson K, Russ SA, Nelson BB, et al. Cognitive ability at kindergarten entry and socioeconomic status. Pediatrics 2015; 135:e440.
  13. Feldman HM. The Importance of Language-Learning Environments to Child Language Outcomes. Pediatrics 2019; 144.
  14. Madigan S, Prime H, Graham SA, et al. Parenting Behavior and Child Language: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2019; 144.
  15. Klass P, Mendelsohn AL, Hutton JS, et al. Literacy Promotion: An Essential Component of Primary Care Pediatric Practice: Technical Report. Pediatrics 2024; 154.
  16. Clay MM. The reading behavior of five-year old children: A research report. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies 1967; 2:11.
  17. Goodman KS, Goodman YM. Learning to read is natural. In: Theory and Practice of Early Reading, Resnick LB, Weaver P (Eds), Hilldale, Erlbaum, NJ 1979.
  18. McCormick CE, Mason JM. Intervention procedures for increasing preschool children's interest in and knowledge about reading. In: Literacy: Writing and Reading, Teale W, Sulzby E (Eds), Ablex Publishing Corp, Norwood, NJ 1986.
  19. Williams PG, Lerner MA, COUNCIL ON EARLY CHILDHOOD, COUNCIL ON SCHOOL HEALTH. School Readiness. Pediatrics 2019; 144.
  20. US Department of Education. America's Kindergartners. Available at: http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2000070 (Accessed on May 27, 2008).
  21. Mulligan GM, Hastedt S, McCarroll JC. First-time kindergartners in 2010-11: First findings from the kindergarten rounds of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 2010-11 (ELCS-K:2011). National Center for Education Statistics; Department of Education, Washington, DC 2012.
  22. Mulligan GM, McCarroll JC, Flanagan KD, Potter D. Findings from the fourth-grade round of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 2010-11 (ECLS-K:2011). National Center for Education Statistics; Department of Education, Washington, DC 2018.
  23. Stanton WR, Feehan M, McGee R, Silva PA. The relative value of reading ability and IQ as predictors of teacher-reported behavior problems. J Learn Disabil 1990; 23:514.
  24. Duursma E, Augustyn M, Zuckerman B. Reading aloud to children: the evidence. Arch Dis Child 2008; 93:554.
  25. Horst JS, Parsons KL, Bryan NM. Get the story straight: contextual repetition promotes word learning from storybooks. Front Psychol 2011; 2:17.
  26. Wells G. Preschool literacy-related activities and success in school. In: Literacy, Language, and Learning: The Nature and Consequences of Reading and Writing, Olson DR, Torrance N, Hildyard A (Eds), Cambridge University Press, New York 1985.
  27. Scarborough HS, Dobrich W, Hager M. Preschool literacy experience and later reading achievement. J Learn Disabil 1991; 24:508.
  28. Snow CE, Ninio A. The contracts of literacy: What children learn from learning to read books. In: Emergent Literacy: Writing and Reading, Teale W, Sulzby E (Eds), Ablex Publishing Corp, Norwood, NJ 1986.
  29. Canfield CF, Miller EB, Shaw DS, et al. Beyond language: Impacts of shared reading on parenting stress and early parent-child relational health. Dev Psychol 2020; 56:1305.
  30. Martin KJ, Beck AF, Xu Y, et al. Shared Reading and Risk of Social-Emotional Problems. Pediatrics 2022; 149.
  31. Locke J. Pittsburgh's beginning with books project. Sch Lib J 1988; 34:22.
  32. Needlman R, Fried LE, Morley DS, et al. Clinic-based intervention to promote literacy. A pilot study. Am J Dis Child 1991; 145:881.
  33. Sanders LM, Gershon TD, Huffman LC, Mendoza FS. Prescribing books for immigrant children: a pilot study to promote emergent literacy among the children of Hispanic immigrants. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2000; 154:771.
  34. Adams MJ. Beginning to Read: Thinking and learning about print, The MIT Press, Cambridge,MA 1990.
  35. Reese E, Cox A. Quality of adult book reading affects children's emergent literacy. Dev Psychol 1999; 35:20.
  36. Pellegrini AD, Brody G. Parents' book-reading habits with their children. J Educ Psychol 1985; 77:332.
  37. Pellegrini AD, Perlmutter JC, Galda L, Brody GH. Joint reading between black Head Start children and their mothers. Child Dev 1990; 61:443.
  38. Whitehurst GJ, Falco CJ, Lonigan CJ, et al. Accelerating language development through picture book reading. Dev Psychol 1988; 24:552.
  39. Strouse GA, Ganea PA. Parent-Toddler Behavior and Language Differ When Reading Electronic and Print Picture Books. Front Psychol 2017; 8:677.
  40. Munzer TG, Miller AL, Weeks HM, et al. Differences in Parent-Toddler Interactions With Electronic Versus Print Books. Pediatrics 2019; 143.
  41. Munzer TG, Miller AL, Weeks HM, et al. Parent-Toddler Social Reciprocity During Reading From Electronic Tablets vs Print Books. JAMA Pediatr 2019; 173:1076.
  42. Ninio A. Picture-book reading in mother-infant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel. Child Dev 1980; 51:587.
  43. High P, Hopmann M, LaGasse L, et al. Child centered literacy orientation: a form of social capital? Pediatrics 1999; 103:e55.
  44. Teale WH. Home background and young children's literacy development. In: Emergent Literacy: Writing and Reading, Teale W, Sulzby E (Eds), Ablex Publishing Corp, Norwood, NJ 1986.
  45. Leung CYY, Suskind DL, Ladner PH, et al. Early cognitive and language development: What low-income parents of newborns know and do (meeting abstract). Pediatrics 2019; 144:53.
  46. Frankenburg, WK, Dodds, JB, et al. Denver developmental screening text II. Denver Developmental Materials, Inc, Denver, CO 1990.
  47. Zimmerman IL, Steiner VG, Pond RE. Preschool Language Scale-3. The Psychological Corp, San Antonio, TX 1992.
  48. Coplan J. Early Language Milestone Scale. PRO-ED, Austin, TX 1993.
  49. Coplan J. Normal speech and language development: an overview. Pediatr Rev 1995; 16:91.
  50. Capute AJ, Palmer FB, Shapiro BK, et al. Clinical linguistic and auditory milestone scale: prediction of cognition in infancy. Dev Med Child Neurol 1986; 28:762.
  51. Capute AJ, Shapiro BK, Palmer FB. Marking the milestones of language development. Contemp Pediatr 1987; 4:24.
  52. Sturner RA, Howard BJ. Preschool development. 1: Communicative and motor aspects. Pediatr Rev 1997; 18:291.
  53. Zubler JM, Wiggins LD, Macias MM, et al. Evidence-Informed Milestones for Developmental Surveillance Tools. Pediatrics 2022; 149.
  54. Johnson CP, Blasco PA. Infant growth and development. Pediatr Rev 1997; 18:224.
  55. Lewis BA, O'Donnell B, Freebairn LA, Taylor HG. Spoken language and written expression—interplay of delays. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 1998; 7:77.
  56. Stothard SE, Snowling MJ, Bishop DV, et al. Language-impaired preschoolers: a follow-up into adolescence. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1998; 41:407.
  57. Johnson CJ, Beitchman JH, Young A, et al. Fourteen-year follow-up of children with and without speech/language impairments: speech/language stability and outcomes. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1999; 42:744.
  58. Schoon I, Parsons S, Rush R, Law J. Children's language ability and psychosocial development: a 29-year follow-up study. Pediatrics 2010; 126:e73.
  59. Whitehouse AJ, Robinson M, Zubrick SR. Late talking and the risk for psychosocial problems during childhood and adolescence. Pediatrics 2011; 128:e324.
  60. Chiocca EM. Language development in bilingual children. Pediatr Nurs 1998; 24:43.
  61. Bosch L, Sebastián-Gallés N. Native-language recognition abilities in 4-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments. Cognition 1997; 65:33.
  62. Oller DK, Eilers RE, Urbano R, Cobo-Lewis AB. Development of precursors to speech in infants exposed to two languages. J Child Lang 1997; 24:407.
  63. Bialystok E. Effects of bilingualism and biliteracy on children's emerging concepts of print. Dev Psychol 1997; 33:429.
  64. Umbel VM, Pearson BZ, Fernández MC, Oller DK. Measuring bilingual children's receptive vocabularies. Child Dev 1992; 63:1012.
  65. Bruck M, Genesee F. Phonological awareness in young second language learners. J Child Lang 1995; 22:307.
  66. Galambos SJ, Goldin-Meadow S. The effects of learning two languages on levels of metalinguistic awareness. Cognition 1990; 34:1.
  67. Bialystok E. Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Dev Psychol 1988; 24:560.
  68. Genesee F, Nicoladis E, Paradis J. Language differentiation in early bilingual development. J Child Lang 1995; 22:611.
  69. Byington CL, Hobson WL, Olson L, et al. The good habit of reading (El Buen Habito de la Lectura): parental reactions to an enhanced Reach Out and Read program in a clinic for the underserved. J Health Care Poor Underserved 2008; 19:363.
  70. Glusman M, Pabalan L, Weisleder A. Pediatrician and parent beliefs and practices about bilingual language development (meeting abstract). Pediatrics 2021; 147:76.
  71. Council on Early Childhood, High PC, Klass P. Literacy promotion: an essential component of primary care pediatric practice. Pediatrics 2014; 134:404.
  72. Xie QW, Chan CHY, Ji Q, Chan CLW. Psychosocial Effects of Parent-Child Book Reading Interventions: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2018; 141.
  73. American Academy of Pediatrics. Early Literacy. https://www.aap.org/en/patient-care/early-childhood/early-childhood-health-and-development/early-literacy/ (Accessed on July 12, 2024).
  74. Roberts MY, Curtis PR, Sone BJ, Hampton LH. Association of Parent Training With Child Language Development: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr 2019; 173:671.
Topic 617 Version 24.0

References