ﺑﺎﺯﮔﺸﺖ ﺑﻪ ﺻﻔﺤﻪ ﻗﺒﻠﯽ
خرید پکیج
تعداد آیتم قابل مشاهده باقیمانده : 3 مورد
نسخه الکترونیک
medimedia.ir

Dissociative amnesia: Epidemiology, pathogenesis, clinical manifestations, course, and diagnosis

Dissociative amnesia: Epidemiology, pathogenesis, clinical manifestations, course, and diagnosis
Literature review current through: Jan 2024.
This topic last updated: May 18, 2018.

INTRODUCTION — Dissociative amnesia is a potentially reversible memory impairment that primarily affects autobiographical memory [1-3]. In dissociative amnesia, the patient cannot recall important autobiographical information, usually of a traumatic or stressful nature, although more extensive memory loss may be reported.

Dissociative fugue, a subtype of dissociative amnesia in DSM-5 [3], is characterized by sudden unexpected travel or wandering in a dissociated state, with subsequent dissociative amnesia for the fugue episode, and often for some or all of the patient’s life history.

Dissociative amnesia and the dissociative fugue subtype are reviewed here. Other dissociative disorders, including dissociative identity disorder, depersonalization disorder, and dissociative aspects of posttraumatic stress disorder, are reviewed separately. (See "Depersonalization/derealization disorder: Epidemiology, clinical features, assessment, and diagnosis" and "Depersonalization/derealization disorder: Psychotherapy" and "Dissociative identity disorder: Epidemiology, pathogenesis, clinical manifestations, course, assessment, and diagnosis" and "Dissociative aspects of posttraumatic stress disorder: Epidemiology, clinical manifestations, assessment, and diagnosis".)

DEFINITIONS

Autobiographical memory – Episodes recollected from a person’s life, with a combination of episodic autobiographical memory and semantic autobiographical memory [1,4]. Autobiographical memory includes recall of cognitive, emotional, and motivational aspects of events.

Semantic memory – Memory of objects, facts, and concepts, including words and their meaning, such as learning the skill of reading.

Episodic memory – Memory of specific events and their context such as “the first day of school when I tried to read a book.”

Procedural memory – Memory of how to perform different actions and skills, such as riding a bike or tying shoes.

EPIDEMIOLOGY — The prevalence of dissociative amnesia found in population-based samples ranges from 1.8 percent in the previous 12 months to 7.3 percent lifetime.

12 month prevalence

1.8 percent (1 percent male, 2.6 percent female) in the United States

Lifetime prevalence

6 percent, Winnipeg, Canada [5]

7.3 percent, Turkey (all female sample) [6]

The prevalence of dissociative amnesia diagnosed using standardized diagnostic inventories in psychiatric outpatients and inpatients has been found to range from 7.3 to 11.4 percent [7,8].

Comorbidity — The comorbidity of dissociative amnesia has not been extensively studied. Many patients with dissociative amnesia have experienced physical and/or sexual abuse during childhood. Large population studies have described adverse psychiatric outcomes associated with such early life trauma including depression, suicidality, auditory hallucinations, substance use disorders, posttraumatic stress disorder, anxiety disorders, eating disorders, somatic symptom disorders, and personality disorders [9-14].

PATHOGENESIS — The pathogenesis of dissociative amnesia is not known. The principal, widely accepted conceptual model for dissociative amnesia is an epigenetic model positing that the disorder results from genes and other neurobiological and environmental factors that influence genetic expression. Overwhelming and/or traumatic experiences during childhood or later life are believed to be principal environmental factors in the development of the disorder [2].

Genetic — Initial twin studies have not shown a genetic contribution to “pathological dissociation” [15]. Subsequent twin studies reported that about 50 percent of the variance in the development of dissociative symptoms could be accounted for by genetic factors, and the remainder to “non-shared” environmental factors, eg, traumatic experiences [16,17]. (See "Depersonalization/derealization disorder: Epidemiology, clinical features, assessment, and diagnosis", section on 'Neurobiology'.)

Several studies have found associations between genetic variants and dissociation/related symptoms:

A cross sectional survey of 935 adults drawn from the general population found a relationship between dissociation, a history of childhood interpersonal trauma, and genetic polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene (SS 5-HTTLPR genotype) [18,19].

A study reported an interaction among a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) polymorphism, Val158Met, and trauma scores (primarily physical abuse not sexual abuse), and dissociation scores [20]. The Val/Val genotype was associated with increased dissociation in subjects reporting higher levels of childhood trauma. The Met/Met genotype subjects displayed decreased dissociation with increasing self-reported childhood trauma.

A twin study reported an independent association between dissociation scores and the SS 5-HTTLPR genotype variant, traumatic experiences, and depression [19].

Other neurobiological — There is preliminary evidence from neurobiological studies in dissociative amnesia of inhibition by frontal systems of temporal/hippocampal and occipital areas associated with autobiographical memory, although other circuitry has been described as involved as well [21-24]. These frontal inhibitory neural network patterns are similar to those found in experimental studies of post-hypnotic amnesia and memory suppression using non-clinical subjects [25,26].

Environmental — A history of psychological trauma is strongly associated with dissociative amnesia, and is widely believed to contribute etiologically to the development of the disorder [2,27,28].

Dissociative amnesia has been reported subsequent to a variety of different types of traumatic events, including [29,30]:

Childhood physical or sexual abuse [31-34]

Adult sexual assault [35]

Sexual trafficking [36]

Military combat [37]

Natural disaster such as an earthquake [38] or firestorm [39]

Nazi and Cambodian holocausts [40-42]

Torture [43]

The association between traumatic experiences and dissociative amnesia has been found in clinical and community samples of individuals drawn from many different populations, and in studies using a wide variety of designs and data collection methods. Documentation supporting the occurrence of the index trauma has included medical records, social service records, and other corollary sources [18,30,31,38,40,41,44-64].

Studies of varied designs, clinical and population-based samples, and varied types of trauma have found the following characteristics of traumatic experiences to be associated with an increased likelihood of subsequent dissociative amnesia [2,65-67]:

Trauma caused by human assault rather than natural disaster

Repeated traumatization as opposed to single events

Longer duration of trauma

Fear of death or significant harm during trauma

Trauma caused by multiple perpetrators

Close relationship between perpetrator and victim

Betrayal by a caretaker as part of abuse

Threats of death or significant harm by perpetrator if victim discloses

Violence of trauma (eg, leading to physical injury)

Earlier age at onset of trauma

CLINICAL MANIFESTATIONS — In a typical clinical presentation of dissociative amnesia, relatively large groups of memories, perceptions, and associated affects have become unavailable, not just single memories, feelings, or thoughts [68]. The unavailable memories usually relate to autobiographical information, for example [44]:

Who a person is

What he or she did

Where he or she went

With whom he or she spoke

What was said

What he or she thought and felt at the time

Dissociative amnesia can be characterized by localized, selective, or generalized amnesia (table 1) [3]:

Localized amnesia — Lack of autobiographical memory for a specific period of time

Selective amnesia — Partial memory for a period of time, eg, remembering only parts of year when the person was in combat

Generalized amnesia — Person cannot recall the entirety of the life history, usually accompanied by loss of memory for the person’s identity

Dissociative amnesia is characterized by retrieval deficits in both episodic and semantic aspects of autobiographical memory. The ability to remember new factual information is generally intact, as are general cognitive functioning and language capacity. Procedural memory is not typically affected, but can be lost in extreme forms of the disorder [69]. (See 'Definitions' above.)

Dissociative amnesia generally has a quality of clear-cut onsets/offsets, whereas ordinary autobiographical memory usually has a gradient, with better recall of more recent information [70].

Other symptoms of memory loss in dissociative amnesia include:

Fragmentary recall of life history

Unrecalled behavior

Unexplained possessions

Inexplicable changes in relationships

Fluctuations in skills/habits/knowledge

Not remembering people who describe significant interactions with him/her

Brief lapses of memory during clinical interview or other interactions

The duration of the period subject to amnesia has been observed to vary, ranging from:

Failure to recall some or all of the details of the index trauma [71]

Unavailability of everyday non-traumatic memories [54]

Gaps in retrospective recall of long periods of life [54]

Most patients who present with dissociative amnesia have a history of one or more traumatic experiences [27]. Longer gaps in memory have been seen more commonly subsequent to chronic childhood physical and sexual abuse [54].

While there are a number of subtypes of dissociative amnesia (table 1) [2], there are two basic clinical presentations [27,72] (see 'Definitions' above):

Overt generalized or dense-localized dissociative amnesia (Type 1)

Covert dissociative amnesia (Type 2)

Overt dissociative amnesia — Patients with overt dissociative amnesia usually present with a dramatic, profound loss of memory for personal history, often accompanied by amnesia for personal identity. Generalized dissociative amnesia affects all of a patient’s autobiography. Dense localized dissociative amnesia affects some substantial parts of a patient’s autobiography but not others.

Because of the extent of their overt memory deficits, these patients are usually brought rapidly to clinical attention, most commonly in the emergency department or in acute outpatient medical or neurologic services. Inpatients with the disorder are more commonly seen by consulting psychiatrists on general medical inpatient units rather than on acute psychiatric inpatient units.

Patients with overt dissociative amnesia can appear confused, perplexed, and baffled by their deficits, although others appear relatively unconcerned despite the extent of their symptoms. They may recall factual information about the outside environment (eg, historical events or famous faces) but recall only limited information pertaining to their own autobiographical experiences [73].

Dissociative fugue — A subset of patients with generalized dissociative amnesia present with dissociative fugue, involving “apparently purposeful travel or bewildered wandering that is associated with amnesia for identity or for other important autobiographical information” [3]. There are limited systematic data on individuals with dissociative fugue, in part because they may be resistant to being interviewed and may flee again after coming to clinical attention. A panel of clinicians with expertise in dissociative disorders has suggested that many cases of dissociative fugue may be a manifestation of dissociative identity disorder [2]. (See "Dissociative identity disorder: Epidemiology, pathogenesis, clinical manifestations, course, assessment, and diagnosis".)

Covert dissociative amnesia — Based on the experience of clinicians specializing in dissociative disorders, the "covert" form of dissociative amnesia is more common than the overt form [2]. Patients rarely complain of this form of dissociative amnesia. It is usually discovered by careful questioning in the diagnostic interview or in subsequent interviews during psychotherapy.

This type of dissociative amnesia generally presents as a retrospective gap or series of gaps in autobiographical experience, often a mixture of significant localized, systematized, and/or selective dissociative amnesia (table 1).

These individuals often fit the clinical construct of "complex posttraumatic stress disorder" [74,75], where repeated early life maltreatment leads to a variety of deficits in emotional regulation, sense of self, body image, relationships, and somatization, as well as problems with dissociation, self-destructiveness, and posttraumatically distorted beliefs about the self, others, and the world. Patients with this form of dissociative amnesia may have additional dissociative symptoms, such as depersonalization, derealization, or spontaneous trance experiences [2,8,72]. (See "Dissociative aspects of posttraumatic stress disorder: Epidemiology, clinical manifestations, assessment, and diagnosis".)

COURSE — There are limited systematically collected data on the course of dissociative amnesia.

Longer gaps in memory have been seen more commonly subsequent to chronic childhood physical and sexual abuse [54]. In a retrospective study of over 13,000 HMO enrollees, the duration of sexual abuse experienced during childhood was associated with greater severity, frequency, and violence of the abuse. A longer period of amnesia of aspects of early life history, up to inability to recall years of the person’s early life, was associated with greater severity of abuse [76,77].

In our clinical experience, many patients with the acute, overt form of dissociative amnesia respond rapidly to intervention such as removal from a traumatic environment (eg, combat) and provision of emergency, clinical treatment [27]. A subgroup of these patients, particularly those with dissociative fugue, may have prolonged, refractory dissociative amnesia for life history and/or identity. Our clinical experience suggests that these individuals are usually very impaired interpersonally and occupationally.

Our clinical experience suggests that the course of dissociative amnesia during treatment may be positively associated with better social and occupational functioning, the presence of social supports, and lack of major, chronic comorbidities such as refractory substance abuse, eating disorders, and severe health problems. (See 'Comorbidity' above.)

Among patients with dissociative amnesia and a history of childhood sexual or physical abuse, studies have found that recall of previously dissociated memory frequently occurs outside of therapeutic settings. Memories of the abuse were triggered by a variety of stimuli [55,65,71], including a patient’s own child reaching an age at which the patient was abused, a patient’s child being abused, media accounts of trauma or abuse, death of an abusive parent, experiences of contemporary trauma (even minor ones), a variety of sensory cues, and feeling safe in one's life situation.

ASSESSMENT — Clinical assessment of a patient with dissociative amnesia would typically identify a pattern of deficits in autobiographical memory with normal cognitive functioning, intact memory for everyday information, facts, and skills, normal orientation to current circumstances, and normal interpersonal grasp of the interview. A comprehensive psychiatric assessment, including a mental status exam, and a medical history and physical exam, should be done to rule out medical conditions, substance use disorders, and cognitive disorders such as dementias and delirium prior to diagnosing dissociative amnesia. Brain imaging and/or an electroencephalogram may be needed in patients with focal findings on the neurologic exam.

In more complex or ambiguous cases, a detailed review of the patient’s medical and psychiatric records may be needed as well as consultation with corollary sources of the patient’s history to rule out factors such as substance abuse, factitious or malingered amnesia, and co-morbid brain injury [78].

Questions useful for testing memory in cases of suspected dissociative amnesia include:

Do you have gaps in your memory of your life?

Are you missing parts of your memory for your life history?

Are you missing memories of some important events in your life, for example, weddings, birthdays, graduations, pregnancies, birth of children?

Do you remember some parts of your life better than others? As an example, when you were growing up, do you recall home better or school better?

Do you “lose” periods of time or have gaps in your experience of time? What is the longest period of time you have lost, eg, minutes, hours, days, weeks, months, years?

Do you ever have blackouts or blank spells?

A careful sequential history may be needed to identity periods of amnesia, to distinguish between dissociation and substance use, and sort out other more clinically complex clinical presentations. A more systematic inquiry may include asking for the patient’s first memory, then asking for subsequent memories in turn. This may reveal significant gaps in the person’s autobiographical memory. In patients who appear to have problems with memory for childhood, the clinician can ask the names of the patient’s teachers and schools. Patients with dissociative amnesia may not recall many of their teachers in early school years, or even what occurred at school.

It is helpful to ask patients for specific, detailed examples when the patient gives a positive answer one of these queries. The patient should be asked if he or she was intoxicated on alcohol or drugs during some or all of the time that they report memory problems. Drug, alcohol, and dissociative memory problems may co-exist in the same person, and some dissociative amnesia patients may also have cognitive disorders related to traumatic brain injury. Some patients with dissociative amnesia rationalize their amnesia by ascribing it to substance abuse.

Patients should be assessed for other dissociative symptoms including (see "Dissociative aspects of posttraumatic stress disorder: Epidemiology, clinical manifestations, assessment, and diagnosis", section on 'Concepts and definitions'):

Depersonalization – Detachment or estrangement from one’s self

Derealization – The sense that the external world is strange or unreal

Spontaneous trance experiences

Dissociative amnesia may be associated with memory that is derealized, ie, the person does not experience memory as first hand, but as something that they “know” about, as if they read it in a book. Some may only know about their childhood history from what relatives tell them, or from looking at family photo albums. It may be helpful to ask if the person “knows” information about the past as if “You were there”, or do you more “know about it”, “were told about it” by your family or friends, or it feels like “you read about it in the paper, or saw it on television”.

Rating scales — The Dissociative Experiences Scale-Revised (DES-R) can be used to assess the initial severity of dissociative amnesia and monitor the response to treatment over time. The DES-R is a 28-item patient self-administered scale (table 2) that can be used for assessing and monitoring the severity of amnesia, absorption, identity alteration, and depersonalization/derealization [79,80].

DIAGNOSIS — DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for dissociative amnesia are as follows [3]:

A. An inability to recall important autobiographical information, usually of a traumatic or stressful nature, that is inconsistent with ordinary forgetting.

Note: Dissociative amnesia most often consists of localized or selective amnesia for a specific event or events, or generalized amnesia for identity and life history.

B. The symptoms cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning.

C. The disturbance is not attributable to the physiological effects of a substance (eg, alcohol or other drug of abuse, a medication) or a neurological or other medical condition (eg, partial complex seizures, transient global amnesia, sequelae of a closed head injury/traumatic brain injury, other neurological condition).

D. The disturbance is not better explained by dissociative identity disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, acute stress disorder, somatic symptom disorder, or major or mild neurocognitive disorder.

Specifier — Specify if: with dissociative fugue, ie, apparently purposeful travel or bewildered wandering that is associated with amnesia for identity or for other important autobiographical information.

Differential diagnosis — The differential diagnosis of dissociative amnesia includes normal limitations in autobiographic memory, a cognitive disorder, acute and posttraumatic stress disorder, other dissociative disorders, substance use/disorders, traumatic brain injury, and factious disorder/malingering (table 3).

Normal autobiographical memory — Normal limitations in autobiographical memory typically include:

Amnesia in adults for pre-verbal memories.

Fewer memories of life before age five or six.

A gradient for recall with more recent events more likely to be remembered and memories of earlier events diminishing with time.

Amnesia for sleep and dreaming.

The gradient for recall seen in normal memory contrasts with a common presentation of dissociative amnesia as having a distinct boundary, onset, and offset, eg, a lack of memory for a specific age, such as fourth grade, or a lack of recall relating to a particular person, activity, or time of year [81].

Cognitive disorders — In cognitive disorders such as delirium and the dementias, autobiographical memory problems are embedded in a broad set of other cognitive and memory problems, as well as deterioration of the personality [2,65,82]. Dissociative amnesia affects autobiographical memory preferentially and does not affect the ability to learn new cognitive information or perform tasks. (See "Evaluation of cognitive impairment and dementia", section on 'Evaluation' and "Diagnosis of delirium and confusional states".)

Memory problems in cognitive disorders are generally irreversible. In dissociative amnesia, the memories may begin to remit once the individual is removed from acutely traumatizing circumstances (eg, combat) [2,83] or may potentially be reversed through treatment [84,85].

Acute and posttraumatic stress disorder — Dissociative amnesia can share certain features with acute stress disorder (ASD) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) — eg, a history of trauma, amnesia for the traumatic event, and symptoms of numbing and dissociation. However, symptoms of hyperarousal and avoidance are not characteristic of dissociative amnesia, and memory loss in that disorder is typically far more extensive and pervasive than in ASD/PTSD. (See "Acute stress disorder in adults: Epidemiology, clinical features, assessment, and diagnosis" and "Posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: Epidemiology, pathophysiology, clinical features, assessment, and diagnosis".)

Other dissociative disorders — Patients with other dissociative disorders may have amnestic features similar to those of dissociative amnesia, but have other clinical features distinguishing them from dissociative amnesia.

To be diagnosed with dissociative identity disorder (DID), a person would additionally have to experience two or more distinct identity states with differences in memory, affect, or clinical presentation among the identities. In contrast to dissociative amnesia, a patient with DID would experience intrusion of material from dissociated aspects of identity [86]. (See "Dissociative identity disorder: Epidemiology, pathogenesis, clinical manifestations, course, assessment, and diagnosis".)

In depersonalization disorder (DPD), the patient generally has intact memory for life history, but experiences repeated or chronic states of depersonalization and derealization. Studies of DPD patients have found reports of childhood verbal and emotional abuse, but not the high rates of childhood physical and sexual abuse as reported for the dissociative amnesia and DID patients [87]. (See "Depersonalization/derealization disorder: Epidemiology, clinical features, assessment, and diagnosis".)

Substance use disorders — Amnesia can result from intoxication or chronic use of numerous substances, including alcohol, benzodiazepines, and marijuana. In most cases, a careful chronologic history of memory impairment and of periods of intoxication can help distinguish between substance-induced amnesia and dissociative amnesia. A period of abstinence for weeks or (in cases of more severe or longer-lasting substance use) months may be needed prior to assessment of the patient for dissociative amnesia.

Co-occurring substance abuse can complicate diagnosis in some patients with dissociative amnesia. It is common for patients with both disorders to ascribe their memory problems to substance-related "black outs". However, a careful history may reveal that at least some of the autobiographical memory problems relate to periods of sobriety (eg, in childhood or adolescence before substance use began).

Amnesia from chronic substance abuse/dependence can usually be identified by presence of additional signs and symptoms. As an example, the amnesia of Wernicke’s encephalopathy is typically accompanied by disorientation, confabulation, inability to learn new information, and often by oculomotor dysfunction and/or gait disturbance. (See "Wernicke encephalopathy", section on 'Diagnosis'.)

Factitious disorder or malingering — Dissociative amnesia should be distinguished from a factitious disorder or malingering [82], presentations in which the symptoms of amnesia are feigned. No test or procedure reliably distinguishes dissociative amnesia from these disorders [88].

Factitious or malingered amnesia is more common in individuals presenting with the following [27,89,90]:

Acute, florid forms of amnesia

A context of financial, sexual, or legal problems

A wish to escape from combat or similar extremely stressful circumstances

These factors may also be present in patients with non-feigned dissociative amnesia [91]. Detailed review of medical history and obtaining history from additional informants can aid diagnosis. Additional information about factitious order is discussed separately. (See "Factitious disorder imposed on self (Munchausen syndrome)".)

SOCIETY GUIDELINE LINKS — Links to society and government-sponsored guidelines from selected countries and regions around the world are provided separately. (See "Society guideline links: Dissociative disorders".)

SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Dissociative amnesia is a potentially reversible memory impairment that primarily affects autobiographical memory. Patients with the disorder cannot recall important personal information, usually of a traumatic or stressful nature. (See 'Introduction' above.)

The 12 month prevalence of dissociative disorders in the US general population has been estimated to be 1 percent in males and 2.6 percent in females. (See 'Epidemiology' above.)

While the pathogenesis of dissociative amnesia is unknown, genetic and environmental factors — particularly trauma — are believed to play a role in the development of the disorder. Dissociative amnesia has been strongly associated with a history of traumatic experiences including childhood physical or sexual abuse, military combat, or natural disasters. (See 'Pathogenesis' above.)

Dissociative amnesia typically manifests with relatively large groups of autobiographical memories, perceptions, and associated affects that have become unavailable. Deficits are seen in episodic and semantic, but usually not in procedural memory. The ability to remember new factual information is generally intact, as are general cognitive functioning and language capacity. (See 'Clinical manifestations' above and 'Definitions' above.)

Patients with overt dissociative amnesia usually present with a dramatic, profound loss of memory for personal history, often accompanied by amnesia for personal identity. In patients with the covert form of the disorder, the memory deficit is typically a gap or series of gaps in autobiographical experience that become apparent with careful questioning in the diagnostic interview. (See 'Overt dissociative amnesia' above and 'Covert dissociative amnesia' above.)

Assessment for dissociative amnesia should rule out medical conditions, substance use disorders (SUD), traumatic brain injury, and cognitive disorders such as the dementias and delirium as the cause of memory problems. Patients with dissociative amnesia may have a co-occurring substance use or mood disorder. (See 'Assessment' above and 'Diagnosis' above.)

  1. Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol Rev 2000; 107:261.
  2. Spiegel D, Loewenstein RJ, Lewis-Fernández R, et al. Dissociative disorders in DSM-5. Depress Anxiety 2011; 28:824.
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), American Psychiatric Association, 2013.
  4. Kihlstrom JF. ‘So that we might have roses in December’: The functions of autobiographical memory. Appl Cogn Psychol 2009; 23:1179.
  5. Ross CA. Epidemiology of multiple personality disorder and dissociation. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1991; 14:503.
  6. Sar V, Akyüz G, Doğan O. Prevalence of dissociative disorders among women in the general population. Psychiatry Res 2007; 149:169.
  7. Foote B, Smolin Y, Kaplan M, et al. Prevalence of dissociative disorders in psychiatric outpatients. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:623.
  8. Ross C, Duffy C, Ellason JW. Prevalence, reliability, and validity of dissociative disorders in an inpatient setting. J Trauma 2002; 3:7.
  9. Kessler RC, Demler O, Frank RG, et al. Prevalence and treatment of mental disorders, 1990 to 2003. N Engl J Med 2005; 352:2515.
  10. Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund PA, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:113.
  11. McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication II: associations with persistence of DSM-IV disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:124.
  12. Kessler RC. Posttraumatic stress disorder: the burden to the individual and to society. J Clin Psychiatry 2000; 61 Suppl 5:4.
  13. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med 1998; 14:245.
  14. Felitti VJ, Anda RF. The relationship of adverse childhood experiences to adult medical disease, psychiatric disorders and sexual behavior. In: The Hidden Epidemic: The Impact of Early Life Trauma on Health and Disease, Lanius RA, Vermetten E, Pain C (Eds), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK 2010. p.77.
  15. Waller NG, Ross CA. The prevalence and biometric structure of pathological dissociation in the general population: taxometric and behavior genetic findings. J Abnorm Psychol 1997; 106:499.
  16. Becker-Blease KA, Deater-Deckard K, Eley T, et al. A genetic analysis of individual differences in dissociative behaviors in childhood and adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004; 45:522.
  17. Jang KL, Paris J, Zweig-Frank H, Livesley WJ. Twin study of dissociative experience. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998; 186:345.
  18. Briere J, Elliott DM. Prevalence and psychological sequelae of self-reported childhood physical and sexual abuse in a general population sample of men and women. Child Abuse Negl 2003; 27:1205.
  19. Pieper S, Out D, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. Behavioral and molecular genetics of dissociation: the role of the serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR). J Trauma Stress 2011; 24:373.
  20. Lochner C, Seedat S, Hemmings SM, et al. Investigating the possible effects of trauma experiences and 5-HTT on the dissociative experiences of patients with OCD using path analysis and multiple regression. Neuropsychobiology 2007; 56:6.
  21. Stanilou A, Vitcu I, Markowitsch HJ. Neuroimaging and dissociative disorders. In: Advances in Brain Imaging, Chaudary V (Ed), InTech, Rijeka 2012.
  22. Markowitsch HJ, Kessler J, Weber-Luxenburger G, et al. Neuroimaging and behavioral correlates of recovery from mnestic block syndrome and other cognitive deteriorations. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 2000; 13:60.
  23. Kikuchi H, Fujii T, Abe N, et al. Memory repression: brain mechanisms underlying dissociative amnesia. J Cogn Neurosci 2010; 22:602.
  24. Hennig-Fast K, Meister F, Frodl T, et al. A case of persistent retrograde amnesia following a dissociative fugue: neuropsychological and neurofunctional underpinnings of loss of autobiographical memory and self-awareness. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46:2993.
  25. Mendelsohn A, Chalamish Y, Solomonovich A, Dudai Y. Mesmerizing memories: brain substrates of episodic memory suppression in posthypnotic amnesia. Neuron 2008; 57:159.
  26. Anderson MC, Ochsner KN, Kuhl B, et al. Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science 2004; 303:232.
  27. Loewenstein RJ. Psychogenic amnesia and psychogenic fugue: A comprehensive review. In: American Psychiatric Press Annual Review of Psychiatry, Tasman A, Goldfinger S (Eds), American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC 1991. Vol 10, p.189.
  28. Loewenstein RJ. Diagnosis, epidemiology, clinical course, treatment, and cost effectiveness of treatment for dissociative disorders and multiple personality disorder: Report submitted to the Clinton administration task force on health care financing reform. Dissociation 1994; 7:3.
  29. Brown D, Scheflin AW, Hammond DC. Memory, Trauma, Treatment, and the Law, Norton and Company, New York City 1998.
  30. Dalenberg CJ, Palesh OG. Scientific progress and methodological issues in the study of recovered and false memories of trauma. In: The Hidden Epidemic: The Impact of Early Life Trauma on Health and Disease, Lanius RA, Vermetten E, Pain C (Eds), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK 2010. p.225.
  31. Williams LM. Recall of childhood trauma: a prospective study of women's memories of child sexual abuse. J Consult Clin Psychol 1994; 62:1167.
  32. Elliott DM, Briere J. Posttraumatic stress associated with delayed recall of sexual abuse: a general population study. J Trauma Stress 1995; 8:629.
  33. Elliott DM. Traumatic events: prevalence and delayed recall in the general population. J Consult Clin Psychol 1997; 65:811.
  34. Briere J, Conte J. Self-reported amnesia for abuse in adults molested as children. J Trauma Stress 1993; 6:21.
  35. Christianson SA, Engelberg E. Remembering and forgetting traumatic experiences: A matter of survival. In: Recovered Memories and False Memories, Conway MA (Ed), Oxford University Press, Oxford 1997. p.230.
  36. Ross CA, Farley M, Schwartz HL. Dissociation among women in prostitution. Journal of Trauma Practice 2004; 2:199.
  37. Bremner JD, Southwick S, Brett E, et al. Dissociation and posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam combat veterans. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:328.
  38. Cardeña E, Spiegel D. Dissociative reactions to the San Francisco Bay Area earthquake of 1989. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:474.
  39. Koopman C, Classen C, Spiegel D. Predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms among survivors of the Oakland/Berkeley, Calif., firestorm. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:888.
  40. Kuch K, Cox BJ. Symptoms of PTSD in 124 survivors of the Holocaust. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:337.
  41. Carlson EB, Rosser-Hogan R. Trauma experiences, posttraumatic stress, dissociation, and depression in Cambodian refugees. Am J Psychiatry 1991; 148:1548.
  42. Carlson EB, Rosser-Hogan R. Mental health status of Cambodian refugees ten years after leaving their homes. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1993; 63:223.
  43. van der Kolk BA, Fisler R. Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: overview and exploratory study. J Trauma Stress 1995; 8:505.
  44. Sivers H, Schooler JW, Freyd JJ. Recovered memories. In: Encyclopedia of the Human Brain, Ramachandran VS (Ed), Elsevier Science, New York City 2002. p.169.
  45. Putnam FW, Helmers K, Horowitz LA, Trickett PK. Hypnotizability and dissociativity in sexually abused girls. Child Abuse Negl 1995; 19:645.
  46. Brewin CR. Remembering and forgetting. In: Handbook of PTSD: Science and Practice, Friedman MJ, Keane TM, Resick PA (Eds), Guilford Press, New York City 2007. p.116.
  47. Widom CS, Shepard RL. Accuracy of adult recollections of childhood victimization: Part 1. Childhood physical abuse. J Psychiatr Res 1996; 9:34.
  48. Widom CS, Morris S. Accuracy of adult recollections of childhood victimization: Part 2. Childhood sexual abuse. J Psychiatr Res 1997; 8:412.
  49. Sargant W, Slater E. Amnesic Syndromes in War: (Section of Psychiatry). Proc R Soc Med 1941; 34:757.
  50. Henderson JL, Moore M. The psychoneuroses of war. N Engl J Med 1944; 230:273.
  51. Trickett PK, McBride-Chang C, Putnam FW. The classroom performance and behavior of sexually abused females. Dev Psychopathol 1994; 6:183.
  52. FISHER C. Amnesic states in war neuroses; the psychogenesis of fugues. Psychoanal Q 1945; 14:437.
  53. Herman JL, Schatzow E. Recovery and verification of memories of childhood sexual trauma. Psychoanal Psychol 1987; 4:1.
  54. Edwards VJ, Fivush R, Anda RF, et al. Autobiographical memory disturbances in childhood abuse survivors. In: Trauma and Cognitive Science: A Meeting of Minds, Science, and Human Experience, Freyd JJ, DePrince AP (Eds), Howarth Press, Binghamton, NY 2001.
  55. Chu JA, Frey LM, Ganzel BL, Matthews JA. Memories of childhood abuse: dissociation, amnesia, and corroboration. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:749.
  56. Loftus EF. The reality of repressed memories. Am Psychol 1993; 48:518.
  57. Geraerts E, Schooler JW, Merckelbach H, et al. The reality of recovered memories: corroborating continuous and discontinuous memories of childhood sexual abuse. Psychol Sci 2007; 18:564.
  58. Myers CS. Contributions to the study of shell-shock. Lancet 1916; 65.
  59. Rivers HR. The repression of war experience. Lancet 1918; 173.
  60. Jung CG. The question of the therapeutic value of "abreaction". Br J Med Psychol 1921-1922; 2:15.
  61. Waelde LC, Silvern L, Fairbank JA. A taxometric investigation of dissociation in Vietnam veterans. J Trauma Stress 2005; 18:359.
  62. Futterman S, Pumpian-Mindlin E. Traumatic war neuroses five years later. Am J Psychiatry 1951; 107:401.
  63. Geraerts E, Schooler JW, Merckelbach H, et al. The reality of recovered memories: corroborating continuous and discontinuous memories of childhood sexual abuse. Psychol Sci 2007; 18:564.
  64. Kardiner A. The Traumatic Neuroses of War, Paul B. Hoeber, Inc, New York City 1941.
  65. Simeon D, Loewenstein RJ. Dissociative disorders. In: Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry, 9th ed, Sadock BJ, Sadock VA, Ruiz P (Eds), Kluwer/Lippincott Williams and Wilkens, Philadelphia 2009. p.1965.
  66. Barlow MR, Freyd JJ. Adaptive dissociation: Information processing and response to betrayal. In: Dissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: DSM-V and Beyond, Dell PF, O'Neil JA (Eds), Routledge Mental Health, New York City 2009. p.93.
  67. Freyd JJ. Betrayal Trauma: The Logic of Forgetting Childhood Abuse, Harvard University Press, Cambridge 1996.
  68. Rapaport D. Emotions and memory, Lippincott Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore 1942.
  69. van der Hart O, Nijenhuis E. Generalized dissociative amnesia: episodic, semantic and procedural memories lost and found. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2001; 35:589.
  70. Rubin DC, Wetzler SE, Nebes RD. Autobiographical memory across the lifespan. In: Autobiographical Memory, Rubin DC (Ed), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge 1986. p.202.
  71. Dalenberg C. Recovered memory and the Daubert criteria: recovered memory as professionally tested, peer reviewed, and accepted in the relevant scientific community. Trauma Violence Abuse 2006; 7:274.
  72. Coons PM, Millstein V. Psychogenic amnesia: A clinical investigation of 25 cases. Dissociation 1992; 5:73.
  73. Kritchevsky M, Chang J, Squire LR. Functional amnesia: clinical description and neuropsychological profile of 10 cases. Learn Mem 2004; 11:213.
  74. van der Kolk BA, Pelcovitz D, Roth S, et al. Dissociation, somatization, and affect dysregulation: the complexity of adaptation of trauma. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:83.
  75. Treating Complex Traumatic Stress Disorders: An Evidence-Based Guide, Courtois CA, Ford JD (Eds), Guilford Press, New York City Vol 2009.
  76. Brown DW, Anda RF, Edwards VJ, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and childhood autobiographical memory disturbance. Child Abuse Negl 2007; 31:961.
  77. Edwards VJ, Fivush R, Anda RF. Autobiographical memory disturbances in dhildhood abuse survivors. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma 2001; 4:247.
  78. Frankel AS, Dalenberg C. The forensic evaluation of dissociation and persons diagnosed with dissociative identity disorder: searching for convergence. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2006; 29:169.
  79. Bernstein EM, Putnam FW. Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. J Nerv Ment Dis 1986; 174:727.
  80. Dalenberg C & Carlson E. New versions of the Dissociative Experiences Scale: The DES-R (Revised) and the DES-B (Brief). Paper presented at Annual Meeting of the International Society, November 2010.
  81. Putnam FW. Dissociation in Children and Adolescents: A Developmental Model, Guilford Press, New York City 1997.
  82. Loewenstein RJ. Dissociative amnesia and dissociative fugue. In: Handbook of Dissociation, 2nd ed, Michaelson W, Ray HR (Eds), Plenum Press, New York City 1996. p.307.
  83. Loewenstein RJ.. Treatment of Dissociative Amnesia. In: Gabbard's Treatment of Psychiatric Disorders, 5th ed, Gabbard GO (Ed), American Psychiatric Publishing, Washington DC 2013.
  84. Cloitre M, Stovall-McClough KC, Nooner K, et al. Treatment for PTSD related to childhood abuse: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:915.
  85. International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociation. Guidelines for treating dissociative identity disorder in adults, third revision. J Trauma Dissociation 2011; 12:115.
  86. Loewenstein RJ. An office mental status examination for complex chronic dissociative symptoms and multiple personality disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1991; 14:567.
  87. Simeon D, Guralnik O, Schmeidler J, et al. The role of childhood interpersonal trauma in depersonalization disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1027.
  88. Ruedrich SL, Chu CC, Wadle CV. The amytal interview in the treatment of psychogenic amnesia. Hosp Community Psychiatry 1985; 36:1045.
  89. Kopelman MD. Crime and amnesia: A review. Behav Sci Law 1987; 5:323.
  90. Wiggins E, Brandt J. The detection of simulated amnesia. Law Hum Behav 1988; 12:57.
  91. KIERSCH TA. Amnesia: a clinical study of ninety-eight cases. Am J Psychiatry 1962; 119:57.
Topic 14697 Version 8.0

References

آیا می خواهید مدیلیب را به صفحه اصلی خود اضافه کنید؟